Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Sleep oscillation-specific associations with Alzheimer’s disease CSF biomarkers: novel roles for sleep spindles and tau

Abstract

Background

Based on associations between sleep spindles, cognition, and sleep-dependent memory processing, here we evaluated potential relationships between levels of CSF Aβ42, P-tau, and T-tau with sleep spindle density and other biophysical properties of sleep spindles in a sample of cognitively normal elderly individuals.

Methods

One-night in-lab nocturnal polysomnography (NPSG) and morning to early afternoon CSF collection were performed to measure CSF Aβ42, P-tau and T-tau. Seven days of actigraphy were collected to assess habitual total sleep time.

Results

Spindle density during NREM stage 2 (N2) sleep was negatively correlated with CSF Aβ42, P-tau and T-tau. From the three, CSF T-tau was the most significantly associated with spindle density, after adjusting for age, sex and ApoE4. Spindle duration, count and fast spindle density were also negatively correlated with T-tau levels. Sleep duration and other measures of sleep quality were not correlated with spindle characteristics and did not modify the associations between sleep spindle characteristics and the CSF biomarkers of AD.

Conclusions

Reduced spindles during N2 sleep may represent an early dysfunction related to tau, possibly reflecting axonal damage or altered neuronal tau secretion, rendering it a potentially novel biomarker for early neuronal dysfunction. Given their putative role in memory consolidation and neuroplasticity, sleep spindles may represent a mechanism by which tau impairs memory consolidation, as well as a possible target for therapeutic interventions in cognitive decline.

Background

With 10% of adults over the age of 65 suffering from dementia [1, 2] and this number projected to double by 2050 [3], understanding the factors responsible for cognitive impairment is of critical importance [4]. Amyloid beta (Aβ) plaques and neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) are two key pathological processes that are thought to lead to cognitive deterioration in Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) Aβ and tau concentrations have been used extensively as biomarkers of AD pathology and found to correlate with plaques and tangles at post-mortem [5,6,7,8]. Emerging evidence suggests that the sleep-wake cycle directly influences their levels in older adults [9, 10]. Specifically, low NREM stage 3 (N3) slow wave activity (SWA) is associated, in healthy midlife and young-old, with trait-markers of high cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) Aβ [11, 12], while active disruption of slow wave sleep (SWS) results in state-dependent CSF Aβ peptide increases within subjects [13]. Sleep disruption has also been associated with increased tau pathology in both animal models [14,15,16,17] and humans [18], but the precise mechanisms are not known. Unlike Aβ, associations between poor SWS and CSF tau have not been reported [12, 13], suggesting that disruption in other sleep oscillations could be linked to tau pathology.

Cortical sleep spindles are 11–16 Hz bursts of activity generated within the thalamo-cortical network that occur during N2–3 and have been defined as slow or fast based on their spectral frequency [19]. A decrease in sleep spindle activity is a good candidate to be associated with tau pathology for several reasons. First, early structures affected by NFTs include sleep/wake regulating centers such as the locus coeruleus [20], suggesting that changes to sleep architecture might be one of the early outward manifestations of tau pathology. Second, sleep spindles are known to decline with age [21, 22], with a specific decline in fast spindles in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD that predicts low MMSE scores [23]. Third, sleep spindles are also associated with sleep-dependent improvement in motor learning [24,25,26,27] and memory [28,29,30,31,32,33,34], with differential roles for fast and slow spindles depending on age [35,36,37]. A decrease in these neuroplasticity-promoting processes could render a greater vulnerability to the spread of tau pathology. Therefore, the mutual associations of cognition with both tau and sleep spindles raises the possibility of ties between them, particularly in older adults. Indeed, NFT load corresponds more closely to cognitive status than Aβ plaques [38], and both AD and MCI patients show significant spindle density reductions [23]. In this study, we sought to determine the relationship between sleep spindle activity in N2 and CSF biomarkers in a population of cognitively normal older adults. We carried out additional analyses looking into other spindle biophysical properties such as spindle count, duration, peak frequency, and slow/fast spindle density. We also explored whether sleep quality measured with polysomnography or habitual sleep duration measured with actigraphy is associated with CSF AD biomarkers.

Methods

Participants and clinical evaluation

Fifty subjects were recruited from a pool of healthy elderly participating in NIH-supported longitudinal studies on normal aging and biomarkers of AD at NYU. All subjects had ≥12 years of education, received the standardized Uniform Data Set II diagnostic assessment [39], and were non-depressed (as defined by Geriatric Depression Scale < 6), cognitively normal and in good overall health. Cognitive tests included to measure declarative memory were subtests of the Guild Memory Scale: verbal paired associates, delayed paragraph recall subtest and the Wechsler Memory Scale Revised: Logical Memory subtests (Logic I and II). A subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale Revised was added to assess working memory (digits backward). The Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) was used to evaluate psychomotor speed. Trails A and digits forward were included to evaluate attention and Trails B Test was included to evaluate executive function. Category fluency (animals and vegetables) and the Boston Naming Test were used to evaluate language. The Mini Mental State Examination was included as an additional global measure of cognition. Cognitive performance data were normalized using z-scores adjusting for age, sex, race, and years of education as previously reported [40]. In addition, all subjects had clinical labs to fulfill eligibility criteria and underwent structural brain MRI. Individuals with documented obstructive sleep apnea (OSA), defined as an Apnea Hypopnea Index with 4% Desaturation [AHI4%] ≥15 per hour, active continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) use, or history of significant conditions that may affect brain structure or function such as stroke, uncontrolled diabetes, traumatic brain injury, lung diseases, drug abuse or MRI evidence of intracranial mass or infarcts were excluded. Both the NYU and ISMMS institutional review boards approved the inclusion of human participants for this study. All participants provided written informed consent.

Lumbar puncture, CSF collection and analysis

Procedures for the lumbar punctures (LP) performed at NYU have been previously published [41, 42]. Briefly, all LPs were performed in the morning to early afternoon (average time of LP was 12:17 pm ± 57 min, and average duration between awakening time and LP was 5 h and 27 ± 54 min). The mean interval between the nocturnal polysomnography (NPSG) and CSF collection was 4.9 ± 6.8 months. Concentrations of CSF total-tau (T-tau), tau phosphorylated at threonine 181 (P-tau) and amyloid beta 42 (Aβ42) were measured using enzyme-linked immunosorbent assays (INNOTEST, Fujirebio, Belgium) conducted at the Sahlgrenska University Hospital (Sweden) blind to both clinical and sleep data.

In-lab nocturnal polysomnography (NPSG)

Sleep recordings were performed following American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) guidelines [43]. Briefly, NPSG consisted of six electroencephalographic (EEG) channels (F3–4, C3–4 and O1–2 referenced to the contralateral mastoid), two electrooculographic (EOG) leads, and one chin electromyographic (EMG) channel. Visual scoring of sleep/wake stages into 30-s epochs of wakefulness (W), NREM 1 sleep (N1), NREM 2 sleep (N2), NREM 3 sleep (N3), REM sleep (R) was performed by a single scorer blind to CSF data [12]. Sleep quality measures including total sleep time (TST), wake after sleep onset (WASO), sleep efficiency (SE), arousals and respiratory events were scored using AASM criteria [43]. AHI4% was defined as the sum of all apneas and hypopneas with ≥4% desaturation divided by TST in hours. AHI-all was defined as the sum of all apneas and hypopneas with ≥3% desaturation or arousal divided by TST in hours. Characteristics of the sleep spindles were obtained from the EEG signals, acquired with a sampling frequency of 256 Hz. Based on the optimal identification of spindles [44,45,46], the C3-lead was chosen for spindle detection. We used our published DETOKS [44] method to decompose the EEG channel into oscillatory and non-oscillatory or transient components. Spindles with duration less than 0.5 and more than 3 s were discarded. Detected spindles were further classified as either fast (13–16 Hz) or slow (11–13 Hz) based on their power spectra, calculated using the fast Fourier transform (FFT). Absolute SWA was calculated using the average power density by FFT in the 0.5–4.0 Hz range at the F4 EEG lead [47].

Habitual sleep assessment

Wrist-based actigraph (Micro Motionlogger, AMI Inc.) data was collected to measure at home objective measures of TST. Subjects wore the actigraph for seven consecutive days on the non-dominant hand and maintained sleep logs to help confirm actigraphy measures.

Statistical analyses

CSF and spindle properties were not normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk test), therefore Spearman correlations were performed for bivariate analysis. Although correlations do not imply directionality, in order to better understand how individual CSF AD biomarkers could be influencing spindle density, we performed linear regression analyses where spindle density was the response variable. To restore normality for regression analyses, a square root transformation was applied to spindle density in N2, spindle count in N2 and fast spindle density in N2, while natural log transformations were applied to WASO, AHI4%, AHI-all, arousal index and CSF variables. Box-Cox transformation was applied to spindle duration and SE. Before conducting the regression analyses, we checked for inter-correlations between all the above variables. A correlation matrix was computed on transformed variables using Pearson’s correlations. Variables that inter-correlated with a value higher than 0.6 (i.e. P-tau and T-tau: r = 0.929, p < 0.001; WASO and SE: r = 0.931, p < 0.001; AHI4% and AHI-all: r = 0.709, p < 0.001; AHI-all and arousal index: r = 0.754, p < 0.001) were not included in the same regression models. Three predefined models of adjustment were used in which spindle density in N2 was the response variable. Model 1 included age, sex and ApoE4 status as predictors. Model 2 added each CSF biomarker individually as a predictor variable to age, sex, and ApoE4 status. Model 3 included combinations of CSF biomarkers (i.e. CSF Aβ42 and T-tau or Aβ42 and P-tau) along with age, sex, and ApoE4 status. After establishing T-tau as the strongest CSF predictor of spindle density, on a final analysis we assessed the extent to which spindle density best predicted variance in CSF T-tau (Models 4 and 5) or the CSF T-tau/Aβ42 ratio (Models 6 and 7) among several alternative sleep physiology variables by performing hierarchical regression with CSF T-tau or the CSF T-tau/Aβ42 ratio as the response variable. Models 4 and 6 included age, sex and ApoE4 status as predictors. Models 5 and 7 added individual sleep measures as predictors including N2 spindle density, frontal SWA, WASO, SE, AHI4%, AHI-all, in-lab TST, and habitual TST. All statistical analyses were performed with SPSS version 23.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY). Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05 using two-tailed tests.

Results

Participant characteristics

A total of 50 subjects (54% female) with mean age 67.2 ± 7.3 (range 53–83) participated in this study. Table 1 lists demographic characteristics of this sample, including ethnicity, common medical comorbidities, and measures of cognition, sleep, and levels of T-tau, P-tau, and Aβ42 in the CSF. All subjects had a Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) of 0, mean Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) of 29.1 ± 1.1, 16.7 ± 2.1 years of education, and were generally non-obese (BMI = 25.4 ± 3.5). Thirty-four percent (17 subjects) were ApoE4+. Overall, the cohort consisted of cognitively normal elderly in good health with 4 of 50 that were both amyloid-positive and tau positive (Aβ42 < 469.5 pg/mL, T-tau > 323 pg/mL or P-tau > 52.8 pg/mL as positive cut-offs), 14 of 50 that were amyloid-negative and tau-positive, 8 of 50 that were amyloid-positive and tau-negative, and 24 of 50 that were both amyloid-negative and tau-negative (Table 1). These cut-off values were calculated based on construction of ROC curves for healthy and diseased subjects (MCI and AD) using the NYU Center for Brain Health biobank. Histograms of the distributions of CSF Aβ42, T-tau, and P-tau in this cohort are shown in Additional file 1: Figure S1). In-lab TST was 6.03 ± 1.05 h with WASO of 91.5 ± 58.3 min and SE of 78.3 ± 11.9% (Table 1). This cohort did not suffer from subjective daytime sleepiness as determined by the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS = 5.9 ± 3.7). In-lab sleep latency was 11.7 ± 12.7 min and latency to REM was 99.2 ± 61.9 min. Median AHI4% was 1.2 (IQR 2.9/h, range 0.1/h to 10.8/h) and median AHI-all was 8.5 (IQR 7.8/h, range 1.8/h to 29.3/h). No associations were observed between spindle density and age, likely a consequence of the relatively restricted age range of our sample. Similarly, although women generally have higher spindle density than men [21, 48], in this sample there were no significant differences in N2 spindle density across sex (males: 1.9 ± 1.3 #/min N2 sleep, females: 2.2 ± 1.5 #/min N2 sleep, p = 0.566).

Table 1 Participant characteristics

Association between N2 spindle density, CSF tau proteins and Aβ42

Pairwise unadjusted and conditional (on age, sex and ApoE4 status) correlations between the CSF biomarkers of AD and N2 sleep spindles density are presented in Table 2. The unadjusted correlations are given below the main diagonal while conditional correlations are given above the main diagonal. There was a high inter-correlation between CSF AD biomarkers, particularly between T-tau and P-tau (r = 0.929, p < 0.001). N2 spindle density was negatively correlated with all CSF measures, and despite the strong correlation between both CSF tau measures, spindle density was more strongly associated with T-tau than with P-tau. The pairwise correlation coefficients adjusted for age, sex and ApoE4 status, given above the main diagonal, show a similar pattern and indicate that the conditional associations were even stronger than the unadjusted ones. In addition to individual CSF protein levels, the ratios between T-tau/Aβ42 and P-tau/Aβ42 have been used as AD biomarkers. Spindle density during N2 sleep was also found to be negatively correlated with the ratio of T-tau/Aβ42 (r = − 0.380, p = 0.010) and P-tau/Aβ42 (r = − 0.31, p = 0.043).

Table 2 Correlation matrix of CSF proteins, SWA, N2 spindle density, and sleep quality measures

To evaluate the relative strength of the association of each of the CSF biomarkers with N2 spindle density, we conducted hierarchical regression analysis with N2 spindle density as the response and each of the CSF biomarkers as the predictors. The results are given in Table 3. The model with only the age, sex, and ApoE4 genotype and no CSF predictors explained 12.8% of the variance in spindle density (Model 1). In the presence of the covariates, each of the CSF biomarkers was a significant predictor, and adding each of the variables improved the predictive power: Aβ42 by 8.8%, P-tau by 18.1%, T-tau by 25.9%, and the T-tau/Aβ42 ratio by 12.9% (see ΔR2 in Table 3), confirming the results discussed above. Finally, inclusion of pairs of CSF biomarkers together (Model 3) showed that in the presence of T-tau or P-tau, Aβ42 did not remain a significant predictor of N2 spindle density, while T-tau and P-tau remained significant predictors in the presence of Aβ42, albeit with T-tau having greater predictive value. There was a dramatic decrease in magnitude of the coefficients for Aβ42 from Model 2, where it is the single CSF measure, to Model 3, where it is together with T-tau (Table 3), while the T-Tau coefficients remained unchanged or increased in strength in the presence of Aβ42. This suggests the principle importance of T-tau among the three CSF biomarkers in their association with N2 spindle density.

Table 3 Hierarchical linear regression examining spindle density as a function of CSF proteins

CSF measures of tau do not correlate with measures of sleep quality or habitual sleep duration

In order to ascertain that the influence of tau is specific to spindles, we investigated potential relationships between T-tau and P-tau and sleep quality variables measured with PSG, including sleep efficiency, WASO, AHI4% and AHI-all, as well as variables measured with actigraphy, including habitual sleep duration (TST). We did not observe any correlations between T-tau or P-tau and any of these measures at baseline (Additional file 1: Figure S2).

To further confirm the specificity of sleep spindles in predicting CSF T-tau, we conducted hierarchical regression analysis with CSF T-tau as the response variable with several sleep measures as predictors (Table 4). The baseline model with age, sex, and ApoE4 genotype alone without sleep predictors explained 7.7% of the variance in CSF T-tau (Model 4, Table 4). Next, we added sleep predictors individually to assess whether or not they improved the predictive power of CSF T-tau (Model 5, Table 4). Of the sleep predictors selected, N2 spindle density significantly improved the predictive power of the model for CSF T-tau by 28.4% above age, sex, and ApoE4 genotype (ΔR2 = 0.284, p = 0.001, Model 5, Table 4), and it was the only sleep variable to do so. The addition of comorbidities including presence of hypertension, cardiovascular disease, diabetes and thyroid disorders did not influence the relationship between N2 density and CSF T-tau (Additional file 2: Table S2). There were also no significant correlations between N2 spindle density and any of the other sleep variables examined in this cohort.

Table 4 Hierarchical linear regression examining CSF T-tau as a function of sleep measures

In a related model, we conducted hierarchical regression analysis with the CSF T-tau/Aβ42 ratio as the response variable with several sleep measures as predictors. ApoE4 was such a significant predictor of CSF Aβ42 that the combination of age, sex, and ApoE4 genotype explained 31% of the variance in the CSF T-tau/Aβ42 ratio. (Additional file 3: Table S1, Model 6). In this analysis, there is no sleep variable that significantly explained additional variance in CSF T-tau/Aβ42 ratio beyond age, sex, and ApoE4 genotype, although N2 spindle density comes closest (ΔR2 = 0.07, p = 0.072 (Additional file 3: Table S1, Model 7).

CSF tau associates with several spindle biophysical properties, but not with SWA

We observed that CSF T-tau was significantly associated with spindle density, count, duration, and fast spindle density (Fig. 1) after controlling for age, sex, and ApoE4 genotype. CSF T-tau was not associated with slow spindle density, spindle power, or mean peak frequency. Fast spindles can be functionally differentiated from slow spindles by their greater tendency to nest within oscillations and promote memory processing [49]. In linear regression models where CSF T-tau, age, sex, and ApoE4 genotype where held constant as predictor variables, we observed that CSF T-tau explained 25% of the variance in N2 spindle count (F (4, 49)=3.77, p = 0.010), 45% of the variance in spindle duration (F(4, 49)=9.23, p < 0.001), and 41% of the variance in fast spindle density (F(4, 49)=7.90, p < 0.001) (Additional file 4: Table S3).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Correlations between sleep spindle properties and CSF T-tau. Scatter plots of N2 spindle density (#/min. N2 sleep) (a), N2 spindle count (b), spindle duration (sec.) (c) and N2 fast spindle density (#/min. N2 sleep) (d) with CSF T-tau indicate significant associations at cross-section (n = 50 subjects)

The cohort examined in this study contains significant overlap with subjects in which we found associations between measures of SWS and CSF Aβ42 (39/50 subjects) [12]. Consistent with our prior observations, CSF Aβ42 was inversely correlated with frontal SWA (r = − 0.312, p = 0.039 after correcting for age, sex, and ApoE4, Table 2). In contrast, while we observed an unadjusted correlation between frontal SWA and T-tau, frontal SWA was not correlated with either T-tau or P-tau (r = − 0.291, p = 0.055 and r = − 0.191, p = 0.215, Table 2) when correcting for age, sex, and ApoE4.

N2 spindle density is associated with neurocognitive measures

Both levels of tau in the brain and sleep spindle density have been associated with measures of cognition. In particular, sleep spindle density has shown to be correlated with the Bells Test, Connors Continuous Performance test, and Auditory Verbal Learning Test when completed the morning following sleep testing [50]. In the current study, neuropsychological testing was completed prior to the sleep study with variable duration between the administration of the tests and the in-lab sleep measurements. Across all subjects, we observed significant bivariate correlations between raw performance values on the digit symbol substitution test and fast spindle density, between Trails A raw performance values and both total and fast spindle density, and between Trails B raw performance values and slow spindle density. When scores on these tests were z-scored and normalized for age, sex, race, and years of education [40], we continued to observe significant correlations between Trails A performance and both total and fast spindle density, and between Trails B performance and slow spindle density (Additional file 5: Table S4). In all cases, higher spindle density was associated with better performance, except for slow spindle density, where greater slow spindle density was associated with longer times on Trails B.

Discussion

Growing evidence suggests that disruptions in the sleep-wake cycle may increase AD risk prior to clinical symptoms. While attenuated SWS has been shown to be associated with elevated CSF Aβ [12, 13] as well as increased PET amyloid uptake [51], other changes in sleep oscillations have not been well characterized with respect to other AD biomarkers. In this study, we investigated the relationship between sleep spindles and CSF T-tau, P-tau and Aβ42 in a population of older cognitively normal adults. Spindle density in N2 sleep was significantly correlated with CSF T-tau, P-tau and Aβ42 both unadjusted and after adjustment for age, sex, and ApoE4. Nonetheless, because of the inter-correlations between CSF T-tau, P-tau and Aβ42, we posit that the association between Aβ42 and spindle density might be largely driven by the primary association of CSF tau with spindle density. This was supported by the loss of significant associations between CSF Aβ42 and spindle density when CSF T-tau or P-tau was included as a covariate.

We interpret the association of low spindle density with elevated CSF T-tau and P-tau as representative of increased AD risk. It is less clear that the association of low spindle density with elevated CSF Aβ42 is also reflective of increased AD risk, given that CSF Aβ42 is lower in bonafide AD dementia subjects when compared to age matched controls [52]. That said, how CSF Aβ42 changes over time, particularly in cognitively normal individuals who may be remote from developing symptoms remains a matter of some debate. For example, individuals with dominantly inherited AD have higher concentrations of CSF Aβ42 on average than controls 20 or more years from estimated symptom onset that then decline more rapidly [53, 54]. Additionally, recent data from the NYU, ADNI and NACC cohorts suggests an early preclinical stage, marked by CSF elevations in tau accompanied by elevations in CSF Aβ42 which were largely observed in younger age quartiles (between 45 and 70 years of age), indicating that elevated CSF Aβ42 might also be associated with very early preclinical AD risk [55]. When viewing CSF Aβ42 in isolation, we cannot rule out the possibility that low spindle density being associated with elevated Aβ42 might be protective. However, it is worth highlighting that spindle density was also significantly inversely correlated with CSF T-tau/Aβ42 and P-tau/Aβ42 which is meaningful because these CSF ratios have predicted decline from normal cognition in cognitively normal older adults over a 1–8 year follow-up period [56].

In addition to total spindle density, we also found evidence that spindle duration and fast spindle density during N2 sleep are significantly associated with CSF T-tau. Of note, a decrement in total spindle density and fast spindle density has also been observed in AD patients, who presumably have high brain tau load [57]. When evaluating other properties of sleep, we confirmed our prior observation that frontal SWA is associated with CSF Aβ42, however, SWA, SE, WASO and number of arousals during sleep were not correlated with CSF tau proteins in this sample.

This seeming specificity of CSF T-tau for spindles stands in potential contrast to recent findings suggesting that measures of subjective sleep quality by questionnaire were negatively correlated with both CSF T-tau/Aβ42 and P-tau/Aβ42 ratios [58]. This may be explained by differences in the use of subjective versus objective measures of sleep used in this study. Several other observations suggest that sleep disruptions from a variety of sources including insomnia [59], OSA [60,61,62] and arousals measured with actigraphy [18] increase risk for cognitive decline and development of AD. Our current results support further longitudinal investigation into whether spindle density is a sleep variable that also increases AD risk. Of note, spindle density did not correlate with general measures of sleep quality in these older subjects without documented sleep disorders, highlighting the utility of multidimensional and oscillation-specific analysis of sleep physiology such as the one used in this study.

While any correlation can represent an epiphenomenon, the observed associations between sleep spindles and CSF T-tau warrant thinking about possible mechanistic underpinnings that link the two. One possibility to consider is that accumulating tau negatively impacts the generation of spindles. Spindles are thought to largely evolve from a thalamo-cortical feedback loop involving thalamic reticular neuron inhibition of thalamic relay neurons. Tau is not thought to accumulate in thalamic reticular neurons, but tau in cortical neurons could affect this circuit. Tau in brainstem neurons projecting to thalamic reticular neurons could be involved [63,64,65], but would be expected to have more diffuse effects on sleep. In any such cases, reduced spindle density could serve as a biomarker of this pathological process [65].

The converse possibility is that sleep spindles reduce the production or increase the metabolism of tau. Animal studies have shown that neuronal activity can promote tau production [66, 67], however, spindles are an organized network pattern, and do not represent a simple decrease in overall network activity [19]. Additionally, sleep in general has been associated with immune function [68] and an increase in interstitial space in the brain [69], both of which constitute plausible mechanisms contributing toward reducing extracellular soluble tau in the brain. To date however, a specific role for sleep spindles in these processes has not been identified. Irrespective of a full understanding of the mechanism, the ability to manipulate sleep spindles with drugs [70] or with greater temporal precision using oscillating acoustic or other stimuli [71] establishes intriguing opportunities to test a causal role for sleep spindles in tau aggregation in human subjects.

Finally, spindle density and CSF tau levels could be associated by being common downstream consequences of an upstream process. For example, tau is released in response to axonal injuries (often from traumatic brain injury) at the time of injury and before frank evidence of neurodegeneration is present. Because we observed associations of sleep spindles with both CSF T-tau and P-tau, we cannot completely infer whether any association is more strongly tied to axonal injury versus formation of neurofibrillary tangles. In subjects with moderate to severe diffuse axonal injury, sleep spindle amplitude and peak frequency were significantly reduced acutely with subsequent return to baseline levels commensurate with functional improvement over 1 year [72]. Tauopathy is also observed in other neurodegenerative disorders including frontotemporal dementia, progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP), primary age related tauopathy and chronic traumatic encephalopathy. If there is a common upstream process that generally promotes tauopathy, reduced spindle density could be a corresponding consequence. In a study that included analysis of sleep spindles in subjects with PSP, spindle density was markedly reduced [73], supporting this idea.

Limitations of this work include the fact that the NPSG can itself alter sleep quality [74] in ways that vary between subjects. However, it should be noted that all subjects completed home monitoring for OSA screening prior to the in-lab NPSG that constituted the source of sleep data. Also, while we strived to perform LPs at a consistent time of day, the time interval between the LP and NPSG was variable and could constitute a source of variance. However, there is low intra-individual variability over 6 months to 2 years in CSF levels of T-tau, P-tau and Aβ42 [75]. Finally, it bears noting that when using a preclinical AD classification [76], our cohort contains a distribution of putative AD pathology, ranging from 48% without evidence for either CSF Aβ42 or tau to 8% that have presence of both CSF Aβ42 and tau above biomarker cutoffs. Therefore caution is warranted in interpreting the prognostic value of sleep spindle density, and studies tracking how sleep spindles change longitudinally along the progression of AD should be insightful.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we report that spindles during N2 sleep are negatively correlated with CSF T-tau levels and to a lesser degree to CSF P-tau and CSF Aβ42. Further, sleep quality measures were not correlated with these CSF measures. These results indicate that poor sleep quality alone does not reduce spindle occurrence and is unlikely to be an explanatory factor in the observed associations between spindle density and levels of CSF tau. While reductions in NREM sleep oscillations, including SWS and spindles, may reflect a preclinical AD state, it remains unclear just how these attenuated oscillations are mechanistically linked to the cellular processes regulating Aβ and tau. Candidate mechanisms would include patterned activity-driven synthesis or degradation of such molecules or the coupling of such oscillations to neuro-immune function. Longitudinal and interventional studies to assess changes in sleep oscillations as well as AD biomarkers are needed.

Abbreviations

AHI:

Apnea-Hypopnea Index

CDR:

Clinical Dementia Rating

CPAP:

Continuous positive airway pressure

CSF:

Cerebrospinal fluid

DETOKS:

Detection of K-complexes and sleep spindles

EEG:

Electroencephalographic

EMG:

Electromyographic

EOG:

Electrooculographic

ESS:

Epworth Sleepiness Scale

FFT:

Fast Fourier transform

LP:

Lumbar puncture

MCI:

Mild cognitive impairment

MMSE:

Mini-Mental State Examination

N1:

Non-REM stage 1 sleep

N2:

Non-REM stage 2 sleep

N3:

Non-REM stage 3 sleep

NFT:

Neurofibrillary tangles

NPSG:

Nocturnal polysomnography

OSA:

Obstructive sleep apnea

REM:

Rapid Eye Movement

SE:

Sleep efficiency

SWA:

Slow wave activity

SWS:

Slow wave sleep

TST:

Total sleep time

W:

Wake

WASO:

Wake after sleep onset

References

  1. Hebert LE, Scherr PA, Bienias JL, Bennett DA, Evans DA. Alzheimer disease in the US population: prevalence estimates using the 2000 census. Arch Neurol. 2003;60:1119–22. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.60.8.1119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Hurd MD, Martorell P, Delavande A, Mullen KJ, Langa KM. Monetary costs of dementia in the United States. N Engl J Med. 2013;368:1326–34. https://doi.org/10.1056/nejmsa1204629.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Norton S, Matthews FE, Brayne C. A commentary on studies presenting projections of the future prevalence of dementia. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:1. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Cedernaes J, et al. Candidate mechanisms underlying the association between sleep-wake disruptions and Alzheimer's disease. Sleep Med Rev. 2017;31:102–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2016.02.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Blennow K, Hampel H, Weiner M, Zetterberg H. Cerebrospinal fluid and plasma biomarkers in Alzheimer disease. Nat Rev Neurol. 2010;6:131–44. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrneurol.2010.4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Tapiola T, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid {beta}-amyloid 42 and tau proteins as biomarkers of Alzheimer-type pathologic changes in the brain. Arch Neurol. 2009;66:382–9. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneurol.2008.596.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Buerger K, et al. CSF phosphorylated tau protein correlates with neocortical neurofibrillary pathology in Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2006;129:3035–41. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Hampel H, et al. Total and phosphorylated tau protein as biological markers of Alzheimer’s disease. Exp Gerontol. 2010;45:30–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.exger.2009.10.010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Yaffe K, Falvey CM, Hoang T. Connections between sleep and cognition in older adults. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13:1017–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70172-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Bubu OM, et al. Sleep, Cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Sleep. 2017;40. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsw032.

  11. Ju YE, et al. Obstructive sleep apnea decreases central nervous system-derived proteins in the cerebrospinal fluid. Ann Neurol. 2016;80:154–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.24672.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Varga AW, et al. Reduced slow-wave sleep is associated with high cerebrospinal fluid Abeta42 levels in cognitively Normal elderly. Sleep. 2016;39:2041–8. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.6240.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Ju Y-ES, et al. Slow wave sleep disruption increases cerebrospinal fluid amyloid-β levels. Brain. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awx148.

  14. Di Meco A, Joshi YB, Pratico D. Sleep deprivation impairs memory, tau metabolism, and synaptic integrity of a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease with plaques and tangles. Neurobiol Aging. 2014;35:1813–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.02.011.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Qiu H, et al. Chronic sleep deprivation exacerbates learning-memory disability and Alzheimer's disease-like pathologies in AbetaPP(swe)/PS1(DeltaE9) mice. Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD. 2016;50:669–85. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-150774.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Rothman SM, Herdener N, Frankola KA, Mughal MR, Mattson MP. Chronic mild sleep restriction accentuates contextual memory impairments, and accumulations of cortical Abeta and pTau in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Brain Res. 2013;1529:200–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.07.010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Holth JK, Mahan TE, Robinson GO, Rocha A, Holtzman DM. Altered sleep and EEG power in the P301S tau transgenic mouse model. Annals of clinical and translational neurology. 2017;4:180–90. https://doi.org/10.1002/acn3.390.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Lim AS, et al. Modification of the relationship of the apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele to the risk of Alzheimer disease and neurofibrillary tangle density by sleep. JAMA Neurol. 2013;70:1544–51. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.4215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Luthi A. Sleep spindles: where they come from, What They Do. Neuroscientist. 2014;20:243–56. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858413500854.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Braak H, Thal DR, Ghebremedhin E, Del Tredici K. Stages of the pathologic process in Alzheimer disease: age categories from 1 to 100 years. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2011;70:960–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/NEN.0b013e318232a379.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Purcell SM, et al. Characterizing sleep spindles in 11,630 individuals from the National Sleep Research Resource. Nat Commun. 2017;8:15930. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms15930.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Martin N, et al. Topography of age-related changes in sleep spindles. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34:468–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.05.020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Gorgoni M, et al. Parietal fast sleep spindle density decrease in Alzheimer's disease and amnesic mild cognitive impairment. Neural Plast. 2016;2016:8376108. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/8376108.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Nishida M, Walker MP. Daytime naps, motor memory consolidation and regionally specific sleep spindles. PLoS One. 2007;2:e341. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000341.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Morin A, et al. Motor sequence learning increases sleep spindles and fast frequencies in post-training sleep. Sleep. 2008;31:1149–56. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep/31.8.1149.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Johnson LA, et al. Sleep spindles are locally modulated by training on a brain-computer interface. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012;109:18583–8. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1207532109.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Laventure S, et al. NREM2 and sleep spindles are instrumental to the consolidation of motor sequence memories. PLoS Biol. 2016;14:e1002429. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002429.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Cox R, Hofman WF, Talamini LM. Involvement of spindles in memory consolidation is slow wave sleep-specific. Learn Mem. 2012;19:264–7. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.026252.112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Tamminen J, Payne JD, Stickgold R, Wamsley EJ, Gaskell MG. Sleep spindle activity is associated with the integration of new memories and existing knowledge. J Neurosci. 2010;30:14356–60. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3028-10.2010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Holz J, et al. EEG sigma and slow-wave activity during NREM sleep correlate with overnight declarative and procedural memory consolidation. J Sleep Res. 2012;21:612–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01017.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Lustenberger C, Maric A, Durr R, Achermann P, Huber R. Triangular relationship between sleep spindle activity, general cognitive ability and the efficiency of declarative learning. PLoS One. 2012;7:e49561. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049561.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Lustenberger C, Wehrle F, Tushaus L, Achermann P, Huber R. The multidimensional aspects of sleep spindles and their relationship to word-pair memory consolidation. Sleep. 2015;38:1093–103. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.4820.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Mander BA, et al. Impaired prefrontal sleep spindle regulation of hippocampal-dependent learning in older adults. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24:3301–9. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Clemens Z, Fabo D, Halasz P. Overnight verbal memory retention correlates with the number of sleep spindles. Neuroscience. 2005;132:529–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.01.011.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Barakat M, et al. Fast and slow spindle involvement in the consolidation of a new motor sequence. Behav Brain Res. 2011;217:117–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.10.019.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Fogel S, et al. Sleep spindles: a physiological marker of age-related changes in gray matter in brain regions supporting motor skill memory consolidation. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;49:154–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.10.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Cairney SA, Guttesen AAV, El Marj N, Staresina BP. Memory Consolidation Is Linked to Spindle-Mediated Information Processing during Sleep. Current biology : CB. 2018;28:948–954 e944. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.01.087.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Nelson PT, et al. Correlation of Alzheimer disease neuropathologic changes with cognitive status: a review of the literature. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2012;71:362–81. https://doi.org/10.1097/NEN.0b013e31825018f7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Weintraub S, et al. The Alzheimer's disease Centers' uniform data set (UDS): the neuropsychologic test battery. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2009;23:91–101. https://doi.org/10.1097/WAD.0b013e318191c7dd.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. De Santi S, et al. Robust and conventional neuropsychological norms: diagnosis and prediction of age-related cognitive decline. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:469–84. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.22.4.469.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Spiegel J, et al. Greater specificity for cerebrospinal fluid P-tau231 over P-tau181 in the differentiation of healthy controls from Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD. 2016;49:93–100. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-150167.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Vanderstichele H, et al. Standardization of preanalytical aspects of cerebrospinal fluid biomarker testing for Alzheimer's disease diagnosis: a consensus paper from the Alzheimer's biomarkers standardization initiative. Alzheimers Dement. 2012;8:65–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2011.07.004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Iber C, Ancoli-Israel S, Chesson AL, Quan S. The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events: rules, terminology and technical specifications; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Parekh A, Selesnick IW, Rapoport DM, Ayappa I. Detection of K-complexes and sleep spindles (DETOKS) using sparse optimization. J Neurosci Methods. 2015;251:37–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.04.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Warby SC, et al. Sleep-spindle detection: crowdsourcing and evaluating performance of experts, non-experts and automated methods. Nat Methods. 2014;11:385–92. https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.2855.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Wendt, S. L. et al. Validation of a novel automatic sleep spindle detector with high performance during sleep in middle aged subjects. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 2012, 4250–4253, doi:https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2012.6346905 (2012).

  47. Varga AW, et al. Effects of aging on slow-wave sleep dynamics and human spatial navigational memory consolidation. Neurobiol Aging. 2016;42:142–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.03.008.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Huupponen E, et al. A study on gender and age differences in sleep spindles. Neuropsychobiology. 2002;45:99–105, doi:48684.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Molle M, Bergmann TO, Marshall L, Born J. Fast and slow spindles during the sleep slow oscillation: disparate coalescence and engagement in memory processing. Sleep. 2011;34:1411–21. https://doi.org/10.5665/SLEEP.1290.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Lafortune M, et al. Sleep spindles and rapid eye movement sleep as predictors of next morning cognitive performance in healthy middle-aged and older participants. J Sleep Res. 2014;23:159–67. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Mander BA, et al. β-Amyloid disrupts human NREM slow waves and related hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:1051–7. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4035.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Olsson B, et al. CSF and blood biomarkers for the diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Neurol. 2016;15:673–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(16)00070-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Bateman RJ, et al. Clinical and biomarker changes in dominantly inherited Alzheimer's disease. N Engl J Med. 2012;367:795–804. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa1202753.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Fagan AM, et al. Longitudinal change in CSF biomarkers in autosomal-dominant Alzheimer's disease. Science translational medicine. 2014;6:226ra230. https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.3007901.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. de Leon MJ, et al. The nonlinear relationship between cerebrospinal fluid Abeta42 and tau in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. PLoS One. 2018;13:e0191240. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191240.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Fagan AM, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid tau/beta-amyloid(42) ratio as a prediction of cognitive decline in nondemented older adults. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:343–9. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.64.3.noc60123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Rauchs G, et al. Is there a link between sleep changes and memory in Alzheimer's disease? Neuroreport. 2008;19:1159–62. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e32830867c4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Sprecher KE, et al. Poor sleep is associated with CSF biomarkers of amyloid pathology in cognitively normal adults. Neurology. 2017;89:445–53. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000004171.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Osorio RS, et al. Greater risk of Alzheimer's disease in older adults with insomnia. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59:559–62. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.03288.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Osorio RS, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing advances cognitive decline in the elderly. Neurology. 2015;84:1964–71. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000001566.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Yaffe K, et al. Sleep-Disordered Breathing, Hypoxia, and Risk of Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia in Older Women. JAMA. 2011;306. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2011.1115.

  62. Sharma RA, et al. Obstructive Sleep Apnea Severity Affects Amyloid Burden in Cognitively Normal Elderly: A Longitudinal Study. American journal of respiratory and critical care medicine. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1164/rccm.201704-0704OC.

  63. Parvizi J, Van Hoesen GW, Damasio A. The selective vulnerability of brainstem nuclei to Alzheimer's disease. Ann Neurol. 2001;49:53–66.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Ni KM, et al. Selectively driving cholinergic fibers optically in the thalamic reticular nucleus promotes sleep. Elife. 2016;5. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.10382.

  65. Mander BA, Winer JR, Jagust WJ, Walker MP. Sleep: a novel mechanistic pathway, biomarker, and treatment target in the pathology of Alzheimer's disease? Trends Neurosci. 2016;39:552–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2016.05.002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Yamada K, et al. Neuronal activity regulates extracellular tau in vivo. J Exp Med. 2014;211:387–93. https://doi.org/10.1084/jem.20131685.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Wu JW, et al. Neuronal activity enhances tau propagation and tau pathology in vivo. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19:1085–92. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4328.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Besedovsky L, et al. Auditory closed-loop stimulation of EEG slow oscillations strengthens sleep and signs of its immune-supportive function. Nat Commun. 2017;8:1984. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-02170-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Xie L, et al. Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the adult brain. Science. 2013;342:373–7. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1241224.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Wamsley EJ, et al. The effects of Eszopiclone on sleep spindles and memory consolidation in schizophrenia: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Sleep. 2013;36:1369–76. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.2968.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Antony JW, Paller KA. Using Oscillating Sounds to Manipulate Sleep Spindles. Sleep. 2017;40:zsw068. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsw068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Urakami Y. Relationship between, sleep spindles and clinical recovery in patients with traumatic brain injury: a simultaneous EEG and MEG study. Clinical EEG and neuroscience. 2012;43:39–47. https://doi.org/10.1177/1550059411428718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Montplaisir J, et al. Sleep and quantitative EEG in patients with progressive supranuclear palsy. Neurology. 1997;49:999–1003.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Agnew HW Jr, Webb WB, Williams RL. The first night effect: an EEG study of sleep. Psychophysiology. 1966;2:263–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Zetterberg H, et al. Intra-individual stability of CSF biomarkers for Alzheimer's disease over two years. Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD. 2007;12:255–60.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Jack CR Jr, et al. NIA-AA research framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14:535–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

None.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the NIH R01HL118624, R21AG049348, R21AG055002, R01AG056031, R01AG056531, R01AG022374, R21AG059179 and R01AG056682; Foundation for Research in Sleep Disorders; American Sleep Medicine Foundation, the American Thoracic Society Foundation, the Friedman Brain Institute, Merck Investigator Studies Program, Alzheimer's Association grant 2018-AARG-589632, and the Swedish and European Research Councils.

Availability of data and materials

Anonymized data will be shared upon reasonable request.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors read and approved the final manuscript. KK: Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, Acquired and analyzed the data, wrote the manuscript. AP: Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, Acquired and analyzed the data, wrote the manuscript. Ram A. Sharma: NYU School of Medicine, Acquired and analyzed the data, wrote the manuscript. AA: NYU School of Medicine, Analyzed the data, revised the manuscript. ML: Nathan Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Analyzed the data, revised the manuscript. BC, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, Acquired and analyzed the data, revised the manuscript. OMB: NYU School of Medicine, Analyzed the data, revised the manuscript. NJC: Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, Acquired the data, revised the manuscript. MDM: NYU School of Medicine, Acquired the data, revised the manuscript. AEM: Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, Analyzed the data, revised the manuscript. LG: NYU School of Medicine, Analyzed the data, revised the manuscript. LM: Weill Cornell Medical College, Analyzed the data, revised the manuscript. NG: Université de Montreal, Analyzed the data, revised the manuscript. KP: NYU School of Medicine, Analyzed the data, revised the manuscript. ZC: NYU School of Medicine, Analyzed the data, revised the manuscript. KB: University of Gothenburg, Design and conceptualized study, revised the manuscript. HZ: University of Gothenburg, Design and conceptualized study, revised the manuscript. NB: Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, Design and conceptualized study, revised the manuscript. BC: NYU School of Medicine, Analyzed the data, revised the manuscript. DMR: Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, Design and conceptualized study, revised the manuscript. IA: Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, Design and conceptualized study, revised the manuscript. MJdeL: NYU School of Medicine, Design and conceptualized study, revised the manuscript. EP: NYU School of Medicine, Design and conceptualized study, revised the manuscript. AWV: Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, Design and conceptualized study, acquired and analyzed the data, wrote the manuscript. RSO: NYU School of Medicine, Design and conceptualized study, acquired and analyzed the data, wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Andrew W. Varga or Ricardo S. Osorio.

Ethics declarations

Authors’ information

NA

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Both the NYU and ISMMS institutional review boards approved the inclusion of human participants for this study. All participants provided written informed consent.

Consent for publication

Both the NYU and ISMMS institutional review boards approve the publication of anonymized study results from this cohort.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional files

Additional file 1:

Figure S1. Distribution of CSF biomarkers in this cohort. Histograms of CSF Aβ42 (A), T-tau (B), P-tau181 (C) in our cohort of 50 cognitively normal elderly. Figure S2. Sleep quality measures are not associated with either CSF T-tau at cross section. Scatter plots of WASO (min.) (A), Sleep efficiency (%) (B), Arousal Index (#/min. of sleep) (C) or total sleep time (hours by habitual actigraphy, n = 39) (D) with CSF T-tau show no relationship at cross-section. (PDF 1529 kb)

Additional file 2:

Table S1. Hierarchical linear regression examining CSF T-tau/Aβ42 ratio as a function of sleep measures (DOCX 15 kb)

Additional file 3:

Table S2. Hierarchical linear regression examining CSF T-tau as a function of N2 spindle density with comorbidities (DOCX 15 kb)

Additional file 4:

Table S3. Linear regression examining spindle properties as a function of CSF T-tau (DOCX 15 kb)

Additional file 5:

Table S4. Correlations between neurocognitive tasks and spindle densities (DOCX 15 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kam, K., Parekh, A., Sharma, R.A. et al. Sleep oscillation-specific associations with Alzheimer’s disease CSF biomarkers: novel roles for sleep spindles and tau. Mol Neurodegeneration 14, 10 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13024-019-0309-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13024-019-0309-5